Mass Shootings & Video Games

Mass Shootings & Video Games

Once again politicians are claiming
video games are a cause of mass shootings.
In doing so, they are making an
argument that dates back at least to Plato. In the Republic, Plato argues that exposure to
certain types of art can have a corrupting effect on people, making them more
likely to engage in wrongful behavior in real life. While Plato focused mainly
on the corrupting influence of tragedy (which could, he contended, cause people
to fall victim to inappropriate sadness) he also discussed the corrupting
influence of fictional violence. As he saw it, exposure to fake violence could
cause a person to be more inclined to engage in real violence. Plato’s solution
to the threat presented by such art was to ban it from his ideal city.

Image Credit

This argument is not without appeal—people are
influenced by their experiences and it certainly makes sense that repeated
exposure to fictional violence could impact how a person feels and thinks. It
also makes sense that exposure to non-fiction, such as hateful speech, writings
and tweets could influence a person in negative ways. The critical question is
whether the influence of video games can be a causal factor in a person
engaging in violence, especially a mass shooting.

Determining whether video games are a causal
factor in mass shootings involves assessing causation in a population. The challenge
is showing whether there would be more mass shootings in a population if
everyone played video games than if no one did. If there is a statistically
significant difference, then video games can be rationally said to have a causal
influence on violent behavior. So, let us consider this matter.

If video games were a statistically significant causal factor for mass shootings, then we would expect to see the number of mass shootings varying with the number of video game players in a country. While the United States is a leader in both video game revenues and mass shootings, other countries also have large populations of gamers, yet do not have a corresponding level of mass shootings. As such, video games would not seem to be a significant causal factor. To use an analogy, if it were claimed that smoking caused cancer in the United States, yet other countries had large populations of smokers with little or no cancer, one would suspect that smoking did not really cause cancer.

This does not prove that video games are not a
factor—it could be that video games combined with other factors do cause mass
shootings. In this case, we would need to look at the differences between the
United States and other countries to see what factors combine with video games
to cause mass shootings. This does suffice, however, to show that video games
are not the primary driver of mass shootings. Now, suppose that video games do
have a role to play in causing mass shootings. The question that now arises is
the extent to which they cause the shootings.

About
67% of Americans play video games of one form or another.
But the concern
is not with video games in general but with violent video games like Call of
Duty
and Fortnite. While most Americans do not play these games,
millions of Americans do. The overwhelming majority of people who play these games
never become mass shooters. As such, if violent video games do have causal influence,
it must be incredibly limited—otherwise mass shootings would be more common. This
does not, of course, prove that violent video games do not have a causal influence—but
it does show that there must be other factors in play. As such, it is an error
to claim that video games are a primary cause of mass shootings. But perhaps a
case can still be made for it being a factor.

Some politicians have tried to make use of the
method of difference to argue that video games are causing mass shootings. This
method involves comparing cases in which an effect has occurred to similar
cases in which the effect did not occur and finding a plausible difference that
could be the cause. This method is certainly reasonable to use but must be used
with due care to avoid falling into error. The gist of the argument is to conclude
that violent video games cause mass shootings because mass shootings increased
when violent video games were created.

While it is true that the number of mass shootings
does correlate with the number of violent video games available (both have
increased over the years), correlation is not causation. After all, the number
of tech startups has also increased, yet it would be absurd to conclude that
they are causing mass shootings. To simply assert that since mass shootings
increased as more violent video games appeared would be to commit the cum hoc
fallacy—that because two things correlate, there must be a causal connection.
This does not entail that violent video games do not play a role, but more is
needed than mere correlation. As argued above, there seems to be no significant
causal connection between violent video games and mass shootings; they merely
happen to correlate as do many other things.

While blaming video games has political value, it does
nothing to address the problem of mass shooting since there seems to be no
meaningful causal connection between real violence and video games.

My
Amazon Author Page

My Paizo Page

My DriveThru RPG Page

Follow Me on Twitter

Be the first to comment

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published.


*